ace recognition paradigm allows for a large number of repeated measures and, in turn, derivation of calibra- tion statistics for each participant. One consequence of this sample size problem is that future research into how calibration varies over forensically relevant con- ditions is likely to proceed quite slowly. Confidence Malleability The issue of social influences on identification confi- dence and the malleability of confidence have already been mentioned—and these issues are also discussed specifically elsewhere. Some further discussion of these issues is required here, however, to round out the discussion of identification confidence. As has been indicated, the empirical evidence shows that witness confidence judgments are informa- tive about the likely accuracy of positive identification decisions if they are solicited at the time of the identi- fication. But from the time of the identification through to the end of a trial, witnesses may have a variety of further interactions with the police, other witnesses, and lawyers, culminating often in a court- room appearance. Although none of these interactions can have any bearing on the accuracy of the decision that was indicated at the identification test, they do have the potential to influence significantly any subse- quent expression of confidence in that decision. This may mean, for example, that any confidence judgment expressed in the courtroom may be quite different from the one that was made at the time of the actual identification test. In turn, whereas confidence at the time of the identification decision may be informa- tive about identification accuracy, these subsequent expressions of confidence may not be. Some of the key variables that have been shown to influence postidentification judgments of confidence include confirming and disconfirming feedback about the accuracy of the identification provided, for exam- ple, by a lineup administrator or another witness. This feedback may be in the form of explicit verbal feed- back from one of these sources or may involve more subtle verbal or nonverbal cues. Regardless of when and how the feedback is delivered, its impact will be to make a witness appear more credible or believable if it is confirming feedback and thus inflates confidence or less credible if it disconfirms and deflates confidence. In other words, cues that can affect confidence judg- ments but not the underlying judgmental accuracy can render a witness more or less believable to jurors. Thus, a witness who falsely identifies an innocent police suspect may not be particularly confident at the time of making an identification but may be exception- ally confident at some later stage in a courtroom. It is for these reasons that eyewitness researchers have strongly endorsed the collection of any assessments of confidence at the time of the identification—for it is then that the confidence judgments are maximally Confidence in Identifications ———141 C-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:41 PM Page 141 informative about accuracy—and have little faith in the probative value of identification confidence judg- ments that witnesses may express in the courtroom. Neil Brewer See also Confidence in Identifications, Malleability; Optimality Hypothesis in Eyewitness Identification; Response Latency in Eyewitness Identification Further Readings Brewer, N. (2006). Uses and abuses of eyewitness identification confidence. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 11, 3–24. Brewer, N., & Wells, G. L. (2006). The confidence-accuracy relationship in eyewitness identification: Effects of lineup instructions, foil similarity and target-absent base rates. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 12 , 11–30. Cutler, B. L., & Penrod, S. D. (1989). Forensically relevant moderators of the relation between eyewitness identification accuracy and confidence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 650–652. Juslin, P., Olsson, N., & Winman, A. (1996). Calibration and diagnosticity of confidence in eyewitness identification: Comments on what can be inferred from the low confidence- accuracy correlation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22 , 1304–1316. Kassin, S. M., Tubb, V. A., Hosch, H. M., & Memon, A. (2001). On the “general acceptance” of eyewitness testimony research: A new survey of the experts. American Psychologist, 56 , 405–416. Keast, A., Brewer, N., & Wells, G. L. (in press). Children’s metacognitive judgments in an eyewitness identification task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. Lindsay, D. S., Read, J. D., & Sharma, K. (1998). Accuracy and confidence in person identification: The relationship is strong when witnessing conditions vary widely. Psychological Science , 9 , 215–218. Sporer, S. L., Penrod, S. D., Read, J. D., & Cutler, B. L. (1995). Choosing, confidence, and accuracy: A meta-analysis of the confidence-accuracy relation in eyewitness identification studies. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 315–327. C ONFIDENCE IN I DENTIFICATIONS , M ALLEABILITY Eyewitnesses are often asked to indicate how confi- dent they are in the accuracy of their identification and other testimony-relevant judgments. These reports are highly influential in evaluations of identification accu- racy. Unfortunately, eyewitnesses’ confidence reports are highly malleable, easily influenced by myriad variables. The solution is to record witnesses’ confi- dence in their identification and report of crime details immediately, so as to preserve whatever useful infor- mation confidence provides. A witness’s confidence in the accuracy of his or her identification is perhaps the most studied of all vari- ables related to eyewitness decision making—for good reason. Eyewitness confidence is the most intu- itively appealing variable for use in assessments of accuracy. Indeed, it is specially highlighted by the U.S. Supreme Court for use in such evaluations. This recommendation is consistent with lay assumptions of what variables predict identification accuracy: People believe that a confident witness is an accurate one. This assumption does have empirical support. Under certain circumstances, there is a strong, positive corre- lation between confidence and accuracy. For example, when viewing conditions are disparate, a strong con- fidence-accuracy relationship emerges: Witnesses who see a culprit under difficult viewing conditions are less confident compared with those who see a cul- prit under optimal viewing conditions. Other research confirms the existence of a useful relationship between confidence and accuracy. One meta-analysis deter- mined that the confidence-accuracy correlation for witnesses who made choices from lineups or photo spreads was moderate ( r = .41). These investigations are highly valuable in clarifying the maximum possi- ble utility of confidence reports in assessing accuracy. These investigations capture confidence reports under the best possible circumstances. Because no external variables have been introduced (e.g., photo- spread administrator influence), they are, in some sense, pure measures of the extent to which confi- dence is related to accuracy. Therefore, it is possible to think of these confidence reports as the estimator versions of this variable because they derive from fac- tors outside the control of the justice system. For example, the system cannot ensure that a witness has a good view of the culprit. Therefore, to the extent that the quality of the witness’s view determines how con- fident he or she is, the justice system has no hand in a witness’s confidence. In other ways, the justice system has a substantial role in the level of confidence an eyewitness expresses in his or her identification. This influence is driven by 142 ———Confidence in Identifications, Malleability C-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:41 PM Page 142 system variables—those variables the justice system can control. Many system variables have been impli- cated in confidence inflation. This influence has been demonstrated across three different categories of con- fidence reports: current confidence in the identifica- tion decision (e.g., How confident are you in the accuracy of your identification right now?), retrospec- tive confidence in the identification decision (e.g., How confident were you when you made your identi- fication?), and reports about details of the witnessed event (e.g., What kind of disguise was the culprit wearing?). As described below, the malleability of confidence in each of these three categories can be attributed to system variables. Confidence in Reports of Crime Details Although confidence malleability is most prominently studied in relation to identification accuracy, some researchers have focused on its malleability in the con- text of crime detail recollections. In one investigation, eyewitnesses questioned over the course of 5 weeks reported significantly elevated confidence levels at the end of that period, without any corresponding increase in the accuracy of their reports. The same elevation occurred when eyewitnesses were questioned over the course of 5 days. In addition, a manipulation as simple as the context in which a confidence report is given can influence the magnitude of an eyewitness’s certainty. Witnesses who give reports about crime details in pub- lic provide significantly lower confidence ratings than do those witnesses who give reports privately, even though the accuracy in both groups is equivalent. The number of times a witness is interviewed and the con- text of the interview are both variables under the con- trol of the investigating officers to a certain extent. Current Confidence in Identification Accuracy In many crimes, many people witness the same event. Some of the crimes for which innocent people were wrongfully convicted include up to five individuals all identifying the same person. In one of the most elabo- rate empirical examinations of the effect of cowit- nesses, witnesses saw a live staged crime in pairs. Witnesses were then separated for the identification attempt and confidence report. Finally, witnesses were randomly assigned to learn one of four types of infor- mation about their cowitness’s decision. Those who learned that their cowitness identified the same person they did or identified an implausible other reported the highest levels of confidence. Those who learned that their cowitness either identified someone else or did not make an identification all had confidence levels that were significantly lower than witnesses in a con- trol (no cowitness information) condition. Information from a cowitness can also alter confidence in reports of crime details. In one study, witnesses’ confidence in whether an accomplice was present at the scene of a crime changed depending on their partner’s report of whether that accomplice was present. The justice sys- tem has limited control of whether cowitnesses speak to one another. At the very least, cowitnesses should be separated until each has provided an identification decision, complete report of crime details, and indi- cated the confidence in each judgment. Cowitnesses are not the only source of contamina- tion for current confidence reports. Photospread administrators have long been targeted as a potential source of influence in eyewitnesses’ decisions. Initially, concerns centered on the ability of a photospread administrator to affect an eyewitness’s choices; research does suggest this is a worthy concern. Recently, how- ever, concerns have expanded to include the problem of administrators influencing an eyewitness’s confi- dence. In one demonstration of this problem, eyewit- nesses attempted identifications in two conditions. In one condition, the photospread administrator knew who the suspect was. In the other condition, the photo- spread administrator did not know who the suspect was. Eyewitnesses who made identifications under the first condition reported higher confidence in their accuracy than did eyewitnesses who made identifica- tions in the second condition. The influence inherent in this situation is easily solved by ensuring that the per- son administering a set of photos to an eyewitness does not know who the suspect it; the system can control whether this safeguard is adopted. Retrospective Confidence in Identification Accuracy Malleability in retrospective confidence reports is per- haps the most problematic of the three categories, in part because this category is specifically highlighted by the U.S. Supreme Court for use in determining accuracy. The Court indicates that the relevant confi- dence report is from the “confrontation,” suggesting that they recognized the possibility for confidence to Confidence in Identifications, Malleability ———143 C-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:41 PM Page 143 increase over time. Unfortunately, profound distor- tions in witnesses’ memories of how confident they were at the time of the identification are easy to create with postidentification feedback. Witnesses who hear that their identification was correct report remember- ing with greater certainty at the time of the identifica- tion compared with witnesses who heard nothing about their accuracy. Because this effect occurs for eyewitnesses who have made false identifications, the manipulation produces a set of highly confident, but wrong, eyewitnesses. As with the other system vari- ables described above, this one has an easy solution. If confidence reports are collected immediately after an identification is made, eyewitnesses’ confidence reports will not be vulnerable to influence by the photospread administrator. Implications of Confidence Malleability As noted above, there is a nontrivial, positive relation- ship between confidence and accuracy under certain circumstances. However, because confidence is mal- leable, the significant relationship between the two variables can easily be compromised or even elimi- nated. One way in which the confidence-accuracy cor- relation is eliminated is by suggesting to witnesses that they prepare for cross-examination. In such a sit- uation, witnesses who have made inaccurate identifi- cations often inflate their confidence to the point where their confidence is indistinguishable from that of accurate witnesses. Postidentification feedback has a similarly devastating effect on the confidence- accuracy correlation: Witnesses who hear that their identification was correct report equivalent levels of confidence, regardless of whether their actual identifi- cation was accurate or inaccurate. The implications of confidence inflation are pro- found because a witness’s confidence in the accuracy of his or her identification carries enormous weight in judgments of accuracy, often trumping other variables. In one set of studies, mock jurors were provided with information about 10 variables, all of which influence identification accuracy (e.g., the culprit’s disguise). None of the 10 variables influenced mock jurors’ assessments except confidence. In another experiment, jurors who participated disregarded the quality of a witness’s view, evaluating him or her positively as long as confidence was high. This reliance on confidence is unproblematic except that eyewitnesses routinely produce highly confident reports about identifications that are incorrect. Ample real-world evidence suggests that this is a significant problem. Many individuals exonerated by DNA evidence were convicted on the basis of confident eyewitness identifications. Future Research on Confidence Malleability Even though confidence malleability is a well-studied phenomenon, there are many unanswered questions. For example, researchers do not yet know for how long confidence is malleable. Some research suggests that postidentification feedback still influences ret- rospective certainty reports even when it is given 48 hours after an event is witnessed. These results are provocative—suggesting that confidence may be mal- leable for extended periods of time. However, because few studies include manipulations of time, the extent to which confidence is malleable is not well under- stood. The reason for the susceptibility of confidence to external influences is also not well understood. One contributing factor may be that confidence reports are derived from many sources. One other factor is undoubtedly the extent to which the stimulus matches the witness’s memory (i.e., ecphoric similarity). Another factor is the desire of witnesses to determine whether their judgment is correct (i.e., the desire for informational influence). In at least one study, the ten- dency to conform eyewitness decisions to others was highest when the witnessing conditions made identifi- cation difficult (i.e., the stimulus was in view for a very short time) and the task was important. Remedies for Confidence Malleability The most obvious remedy for confidence inflation is to record witnesses’ reports immediately after an iden- tification is made or a crime is reported. This solution is appealing for three reasons. First, providing a con- fidence report may in fact inoculate witnesses against future inflations. In one study, witnesses who pro- vided a private retrospective confidence report were less affected by postidentification feedback than were witnesses who did not. Second, records of confidence reports would allow defense attorneys to challenge subsequent inflation through cross-examination at trial. This is likely to be difficult, as one study demonstrated that mock jurors are resistant to attempts to undermine a witness’s confidence report 144 ———Confidence in Identifications, Malleability C-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:41 PM Page 144 by providing evidence that it has inflated over time. Finally, recording confidence is easy. It does not require specialized equipment or training. It can eas- ily be incorporated into interviews with witnesses. Should immediate confidence reports not be recorded, another common suggestion is to introduce expert testimony on the malleability of confidence. This solution is less appealing because research has demonstrated that mock jurors are relatively insensi- tive to testimony impugning the correlation between confidence and accuracy. In some studies, jurors per- sist in using confidence reports even after being told that they are only minimally useful in assessing accu- racy. Therefore, the most reasonable solution is to pre- vent eyewitnesses’ confidence from inflating in the first place. The best way to do this is to collect imme- diate records of confidence reports in both identifica- tion accuracy and crime details. Amy Bradfield Douglass See also Confidence in Identifications; Estimator and System Variables in Eyewitness Identification; Expert Psychological Testimony on Eyewitness Identification; Eyewitness Memory; Identification Tests, Best Practices in; Juries and Eyewitnesses; Neil v. Biggers Criteria for Evaluating Eyewitness Identification; Postevent Information and Eyewitness Memory; Wrongful Conviction Further Readings Baron, R. S., Vandello, J. A., & Brunsman, B. (1996). The forgotten variable in conformity research: Impact of task importance on social influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71 (5), 915–927. Bradfield, A., & McQuiston, D. E. (2002). When does evidence of eyewitness confidence inflation affect judgments in a criminal trial? Law and Human Behavior, 28 (4), 369–387. Bradfield, A. L., Wells, G. L., & Olson, E. A. (2002). The damaging effect of confirming feedback on the relation between eyewitness certainty and identification accuracy. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87 (1), 112–120. Lindsay, S. D., Read, J. D., & Sharma, K. (1998). Accuracy and confidence in person identification: The relationship is strong when witnessing conditions vary widely. Psychological Science, 9 (3), 215–218. Luus, C. A. E., & Wells, G. L. (1994). The malleability of eyewitness confidence: Co-witness and perseverance effects. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 714–724. Sporer, S., Penrod, S., Read, D., & Cutler, B. L. (1995). Choosing, confidence, and accuracy: A meta-analysis of the confidence-accuracy relation in eyewitness identification studies. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 315–327. C ONFLICT T ACTICS S CALE (CTS) Two general types of incidence surveys exist: the Conflict Tactics Scale (CTS) and the Crime Victim Surveys (CVS). The former requires people to indicate what actions they have taken to resolve family con- flicts; the latter requires people to indicate by what crimes they have been victimized. The CVS find the rates of reporting wife assault to the police comparable with the reporting rates for other assaults. However, these surveys have a filtering problem such that people who do not consider their abuse victimization to be a crime do not respond in the affirmative. Hence, inci- dence rates of reported spousal abuse, which are not defined as criminal by the victim, are low. To circum- vent this filtering problem, Straus, Gelles, and Steinmetz devised the CTS, which asks respondents to report modes of conflict resolution in the family. This avoids the problem of whether the respondent defines the action as criminal or not and, therefore, attempts to obtain more accurate estimates of the frequency and incidence of domestic assault in a general population. Straus found violence incidence rates with the CTS were 16 times greater than with the CVS. Presenting the CTS in the context of normal conflict rather than a criminal act reduces filters against reporting. Surveys of Incidence: Conflict Tactics Surveys Several surveys using the CTS have been completed. They include (a) a nationally representative U.S. sam- ple of 2,143 interviewed in 1974 by Response Analysis Corporation; (b) a survey of spousal violence against women in the state of Kentucky, which interviewed 1,793 women; © a second national survey completed by Straus and Gelles in 1985; and (d) a sample of 1,045 for the Province of Alberta, Canada. These were each obtained by a survey that interviews a representa- tive sample drawn from a general population about experiences of being victimized by violence during family conflicts and the type of actions used to resolve Conflict Tactics Scale (CTS) ———145 C-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:41 PM Page 145 these conflicts. These rates refer to the use of violence at any time in the marriage and may include both uni- lateral and reciprocal violence. Straus reported approx- imately equal perpetration rates by gender. This common measure enables some direct comparison between these surveys. Kennedy and Dutton used a combination of face-to- face meeting and random-digit dialing techniques to survey 1,045 residents in Alberta, Canada, leading to a comparison of American and Canadian rates of wife assault. The “minor” violence rates for the two coun- tries are virtually identical, but the American “severe violence” rates were higher than the Canadian rates. By way of comparison with these North American data, Kim and Cho reported that the Korean intimate partner violence rate was 37.5% for wife assault (any violence) in the preceding year versus 11.6% reported by Straus et al. In 1985, Fumagai and Straus found a lifetime incidence of wife assault in Japan of 58.7% versus 22% in the United States. Fals-Stewart, Birchler, and Kelley asked 104 men in a spousal violence treatment program and their partners to keep a weekly diary identifying days of physical aggression and a daily CTS checklist for vio- lence. Male-to-female agreement on “violence days” was better after treatment than before, perhaps because couples were aware of tracking. Interestingly, the women were violent on more days than the men, regardless of whose report was read. Moffitt et al. confirmed Straus’s point in one of the best methodological studies of intimate violence to date, the Dunedin study. When asked about “assault victimization,” they found that respondents reported rates of male violence that were much higher than the rates of female violence, and both rates were quite low. When they asked the same respondents about “relationships with partners,” the rates reported by both genders were much higher and equivalent. Criticisms Some criticisms have been made about the CTS: (a) the CTS ignores the context in which the violence occurred, (b) differences in gender size between men and women make acts scored the same on the CTS quite different in reality, © impression management or social desirability factors may preclude people from answering the CTS accurately, and (d) the CTS queries violence occurring in a conflict and may miss “out of the blue” violence. Straus’s rejoinders to these criticisms are as fol- lows. First, the assessment of context should be done separately because there are so many context vari- ables that they could not all be included on the CTS. The CTS is designed in such a way that any special set of context questions can be easily added. Second, a similar problem, Straus notes, is that repeated slap- ping is highly abusive and dangerous but gets counted as Minor Violence on the CTS. Straus argues that while it is possible to weight actions by differences in size between perpetrator and victim, or to construct an upper limit after which slapping gets counted as Severe Violence, such weightings have rarely led to changes in research results. Third, the social desir- ability criticism was answered, in part, by a study by Dutton and Hemphill, which correlated scores on two measures of social desirability (the tendency to pre- sent a “perfect image” on self-report tests) and scores on the CTS. Social desirability is measured by a test called the Marlowe-Crowne test (MC), which assesses the tendency to present the self in a socially acceptable manner. MC scores did correlate signifi- cantly; the higher their social desirability score, the lower their reported rates of verbal abuse. However, it did not correlate with their reports of physical abuse, nor with any reports of abuse (verbal or physical) made against them by their wives. Hence, it seems that reports of physical abuse are largely uncontami- nated by socially desirable responding. This means that the incidence survey rates are probably fairly accurate as far as image management is concerned. Finally, the vast majority of violent acts are perceived as emanating from conflict. While the CTS may miss an out-of-the-blue attack, it more than makes up for this with its increased sensitivity over crime victim surveys. Donald G. Dutton, Jessica Broderick, and Makenzie Chilton See also Domestic Violence Screening Instrument (DVSI); Intimate Partner Violence; Spousal Assault Risk Assessment (SARA) Further Readings Browning, J., & Dutton, D. G. (1986). Assessment of wife assault with the Conflict Tactics Scale: Using couple data to quantify the differential reporting effect. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 48, 375–379. 146 ———Conflict Tactics Scale (CTS) C-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:41 PM Page 146 Straus, M. A., & Gelles, R. J. (Eds.). (1990). Physical violence in American families. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction. Straus, M. A., Hamby, S. L., & Boney-McCoy, S. (1996). The revised Conflict Tactics Scales (CTS2): Development and preliminary psychometric data. Journal of Family Issues, 17, 283–316. C ONFORMITY IN E YEWITNESS REPORTS Eyewitness research has repeatedly shown that exposure to postevent information can affect a witness’s ability to accurately report details of an originally encoded event. In everyday life, postevent information might be encountered when individuals who have shared the same experience discuss this with one another. Even when each person has witnessed the same event, their memories are likely to differ because of naturally occur- ring differences in the details attended to at the time, as well as differences in each person’s ability to accurately remember those details. Despite initial differences in recollections of an event, when people talk about their memories they can influence each other such that their subsequent individual memory reports become similar. The phenomenon of people’s memory reports becoming similar to one another’s following a discussion has been referred to as “memory conformity.” This entry dis- cusses the ways in which researchers have investigated conformity in eyewitness reports, typical research find- ings, and current theoretical explanations for the mem- ory conformity effect. When memory conformity occurs in the context of a forensic investigation, there can be serious implica- tions. For example, it might be assumed that seem- ingly corroborative witness statements are a product of independent witnesses with consistent versions of events, when in fact memory conformity might be responsible for the similarities if there has been some form of interaction between cowitnesses. Therefore, it is important that the police take care not to give undue weight to the consistency of statements from wit- nesses who may have talked when judging the accu- racy of an eyewitness account. A typical paradigm used to investigate memory conformity in eyewitness reports involves pairs of par- ticipants being led to believe that they have encoded the same stimuli (often a simulated crime event shown on video or slides), when in reality they are shown stimuli that bear a similarity but differ in critical ways. These critical differences can take the form of added items (where one dyad member sees an item that his or her partner did not and vice versa) or contradicting items (where both dyad members see the same item, but details of this item differ in terms of color or prod- uct). This manipulation allows different features of the encoded stimuli to be observed by each partici- pant. Dyad members are then given time to discuss what they have seen. An individual recall test for the originally encoded stimuli is then administered to examine the effects of cowitness discussion on mem- ory. The dependent variable of interest is whether, and how often, witnesses report an item at test that they have encountered from a cowitness as opposed to see- ing with their own eyes. Alternative procedures to investigate memory con- formity include using a confederate to act as a cowit- ness and purposefully introduce items of misleading postevent information into the discussion. Other experiments have presented cowitness information indirectly by incorporating it into a recall question- naire, or the experimenter reveals responses that have purportedly been given by other witnesses. A common finding for memory conformity research, regardless of procedure or stimuli used, is that social influences encountered in the form of postevent infor- mation from a cowitness can mediate accuracy in joint recall and recognition tasks, with individuals often exhibiting conformity to the suggestions and judg- ments of others. Significant conformity effects are also evident following a delay in postdiscussion mem- ory tests that are performed alone. Theoretical explanations for conformity in eyewit- ness reports share strong parallels with those account- ing for the effects of postevent information on memory. For example, research has shown that source misattributions account in part for conformity in eye- witness reports, as individuals sometimes claim to remember seeing items of information that have actu- ally been encountered from a cowitness. Informational motivations to report accurate information at test are also thought to play a role. Here, individuals choose to report the postevent information encountered from a cowitness at test if it is accepted as veridical. Informational motivations to conform are often evident in situations where individuals doubt the accuracy of their own memory or when the information encoun- tered from another individual convinces them that their initial judgment was erroneous. In support of this, Conformity in Eyewitness Reports ———147 C-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:41 PM Page 147 research has found that the influence exerted by one person on another’s memory judgments can be modu- lated by person perception factors. For example, ten- dencies to conform can be increased (or decreased) by manipulating the perceptions of each individual regarding the relative knowledge each has of stimuli they encoded together as a dyad. Similar effects can be obtained by manipulating the perceived relative com- petence of each individual or the overt confidence with which individuals make their assertions to each other. Research continues to explore which factors can increase, decrease, and possibly eliminate the longer- term effects of conformity on memory. However, progress in addressing such issues has been hampered by the complexity of the phenomenon itself, due to the inherently dynamic and variable nature of realistic interactions between individuals. Despite this, new par- adigms to investigate conformity in eyewitness reports are being developed and refined so that the effects of naturalistic interactions on subsequent memory reports can be investigated with full experimental control. Fiona Gabbert See also Eyewitness Memory; False Memories; Postevent Information and Eyewitness Memory; Source Monitoring and Eyewitness Memory Further Readings Gabbert, F., Memon, A., & Allan, K. (2003). Memory conformity: Can eyewitnesses influence each other’s memories for an event? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 533–543. Roediger, H. L., Meade, M. L., & Bergman, E. T. (2001). Social contagion of memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 365–371. Shaw, J. S., Garven, S., & Wood, J. M. (1997). Co-witness information can have immediate effects on eyewitness memory reports. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 503–523. Wright, D. B., Self, G., & Justice, C. (2000). Memory conformity: Exploring misinformation effects when presented by another person. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 189–202. C ONSENT TO C LINICAL R ESEARCH Informed consent practices have evolved over time after instances were documented in which research participants were not treated fairly or respectfully, were not informed, or were subjected to unnecessary harm. Current federal regulations support the ethical treat- ment of persons in the research setting in that the par- ticipation is voluntary, that the risks outweigh the benefits, and that all people are given an equal chance to participate. When a researcher invites a participant into the research setting, the researcher is required to provide the necessary information, to ensure that the participant fully understands the information, and to stop the research if it is felt that these standards have not been met. Prior to enrollment in a research study, that candi- date must provide valid consent for participation. That is, the participant must be fully informed about the research purposes, risks, benefits, freedom to withdraw consent, and other relevant information; must enter vol- untarily into the research; and must be capable of mak- ing an informed decision. Informed consent to clinical research is important in the field of psychology and law, because psychological researchers must be aware of the requirements of conducting research and must protect themselves and also their research participants from any ethical breaches. Clinical research refers to studies con- ducted in a setting where clinical conditions, either med- ical or psychiatric, are diagnosed and treated. This entry provides a historical basis for the evolution of informed consent, the requirements of informed consent, and will end with a brief discussion of the capacities of poten- tially vulnerable individuals who may have compro- mised ability to give a valid consent by virtue of impaired capacity or lack of voluntariness. Historical Perspective In research settings, individuals are protected by the doctrine of informed consent, which has evolved through policies, regulations, and professional codes. In the 1940s, unspeakable acts were committed when medical experiments on human subjects were con- ducted in concentration camps in Nazi Germany. In response to the atrocious experiments, the first formal document for conveying the ideas of protection of per- sons as human subjects and informed consent was developed. The Nuremberg Code set forth 10 guide- lines for the ethical treatment of persons involved in research. The first statement of the Nuremberg Code states that the voluntary consent of the human subject is absolutely essential. Before an affirmative decision by the person can be made, one must know the nature, duration, and purpose of the experiment; the method and means in which it is conducted; and the reason- able inconveniences expected, which may possibly 148 ———Consent to Clinical Research C-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:41 PM Page 148 come from participation. The Nuremberg Code fur- ther states that experiments should only be conducted by scientifically qualified persons, that the results should yield fruitful results for the good of society, that all harm to the participant should be avoided, and that the participant may end the experiment at any time. The Nuremberg Code was expanded when the World Medical Association adopted the Declaration of Helsinki in 1964. The Code established indepen- dent ethical review committees to oversee all experi- mental procedures, which set the stage for the later development of institutional review boards (IRBs). Although the Nuremburg Code helped to identify basic ethical principles of research, there were still instances in which egregious ethical breaches contin- ued. The Tuskegee Syphilis Study, which began in 1932 and ultimately ended in 1972, was a research study in which medically ill patients were not offered effective medical treatment, which became available during the course of the research. In 1972, the uneth- ical manner of the research project was made public and this ultimately led to the 1974 Research Act. The National Research Act was signed into law, thereby creating the National Commission for the Protection of Human Subjects of Biomedical and Behavioral Research. The charge of the commission was to iden- tify the basic ethical principles that should underlie research involving human subjects and to develop guidelines that should be followed in accordance with the described principles. The 1979 Belmont Report emerged as a product of this work group and man- dated that all researchers gain approval from an IRB before proceeding with any type of experimentation. Principles of Beneficence, Justice, and Respect The Belmont Report specified the three basic ethical principles governing scientific research. These include respect for persons, beneficence, and justice. Respect for persons specifies that individuals should be treated as autonomous persons, and those whose decision making is compromised should be given special pro- tections and safeguards. Furthermore, participants must enter into a research setting voluntarily and with adequate information. Beneficence is applied in the research setting in that persons are entitled to partici- pation that is free from harm and that maximizes pos- sible benefits and minimizes possible harm. The principle of justice dictates that individuals should be treated fairly and equally in the research setting, according to their need, effort, contribution, and merit, and that all persons must share the responsibility of research. The three principles of the Belmont Report are translated into informed consent by maintaining that persons are participating of their own free will and that the benefits to the person outweigh the risks. The Belmont Report served as the basis for the Code of Federal Regulations, which was approved in 1978. In addition, in 1993, the International Ethical Guidelines for Biomedical Research Involving Human Subjects were developed. In 2005, the Department of Health and Human Services released a revised edition of the Federal Code of Regulations on the Protection of Human Subjects. Title 45, part 46 of the U.S. Code of Federal Regulations upholds the application of the Belmont Report principles and is now generally accepted as the uniform policy for the protection of research participants. Outlined are specific definitions and statements as they apply to both federally and non- federally funded research projects. Requirements of Informed Consent The theory of informed consent to research is that a research participant is both voluntary and fully informed about the nature and consequences of an experimental situation before giving consent to partici- pate. There are three essential elements to the doctrine of informed consent: voluntariness, information, and competency. The first element is that the person volun- tarily consents to the procedure, in that the individual chooses freely to participate. To be voluntary, the per- son must consent without the presence of coercion, fraud, or duress, which may hinder the person’s deci- sion-making ability. Stanley and Guido further elabo- rate that when an individual is consenting to participate in psychological research, one must consider the envi- ronment to ensure that the participant’s voluntary status is not compromised by the setting (e.g., prison, hospi- tal, school). Second, an individual must be fully informed of the proposed research setting to which he or she is consenting. The disclosure of information, which is provided in a formal informed consent document to the research participant, should include a description of the proposed procedures, its purpose, duration of the research procedure, the risks and benefits of par- ticipation, alternatives to participation, and the volun- tariness of participation. Other issues that should be disclosed are that the individual has the opportunity to Consent to Clinical Research ———149 C-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:41 PM Page 149 withdraw from the research setting at any time, issues of confidentiality, and any other pertinent information such as how to contact the main investigator. The final element required to obtain informed con- sent is competency, which refers to the functional capacity to give valid consent to participate in research. Grisso and Appelbaum note that those persons who cannot use the disclosed information because of a lack of certain cognitive abilities are not capable of partici- pating in an informed consent procedure. When the impairment reaches a certain level of severity, a deter- mination can be made of incompetence. In legal terms, a de facto incompetence determination is made when inquiries into the person’s actual capacities reveal a suf- ficient lack in cognitive functionality. When making a judgment about one’s decision- making capacities, three types of information are usu- ally required as delineated by Grisso and Appelbaum: (1) the person’s clinical condition, (2) the person’s degree of functioning in tasks involving decision- making abilities, and (3) the situation-specif ic demands. There are four leg al standards f or assessing decision- making competence, which include the ability to com- municate a choice, the ability to understand relevant information, the ability to appreciate the circumstances and likely outcome, and the ability to rationally manip- ulate the information. Grisso and Appelbaum also note that although four standards have been proposed, not all courts and jurisdictions apply all concepts in a determination of competency. Also of importance is that a person’s status on the four abilities is not all-or- none. A person usually possesses all the abilities but in varying degrees and one must consider the complexity of the decision being made in relation to the condition of the person making that decision. For researchers it is important to provide all the necessary information to the potential participant so that he or she is able to make an informed decision. The information of the purposes, procedures, benefits, risks, and voluntary nature of participation must be outlined in a written document and signed by both parties. It is the responsibility of the researcher to ensure that the participant understands the informa- tion and is not being coerced in any way to participate. Competency of Special Populations When conducting research with normal healthy pop- ulations, researchers tend to presume that the partici- pant is capable of understanding the material in the informed consent document and capable of making an autonomous decision of whether to participate or not. However, when conducting research with poten- tially vulnerable populations, the same assumption cannot necessarily be made. Potentially vulnerable populations include children and adolescents, who are vulnerable because of their developmental level and their susceptibility to coercion; prisoners and other institutionalized individuals by virtue of their lack of voluntary status; medical patients who may have impaired cognitive function ranging from being comatose to some memory impairment; and psychi- atric patients as a result of possible compromised capacity to consent. Several empirical studies have examined the ability of psychiatric patients to pro- vide informed consent; a brief summary of the find- ings follows. In persons with schizophrenia and psychotic disorders, mixed results have been pro- duced. The consensus is that these types of patients, on the whole, perform more poorly than their non-ill counterparts on tests of competency. Several studies, however, have further noted that even with this divide, there is much heterogeneity among the schiz- ophrenic patients and many are able to perform at a level similar to non-ill persons. Research on persons with affective disorders is somewhat more promising, with this group performing at a level similar to non- ill persons in most of the published studies. Finally, elderly patients with Alzheimer’s disease seem to be the population at the greatest risk for having impaired levels of cognitive processing and thus a diminished ability to provide informed consent. Elizabeth Arias and Barbara Stanley See also Capacity to Consent to Treatment; Capacity to Consent to Treatment Instrument (CCTI); MacArthur Competence Assessment Tool for Clinical Research (MacCAT–CR) Further Readings Appelbaum, P., Lidz, C., & Meisel, A. (1987). Informed consent. New York: Oxford University Press. Meisel, L., & Lidz, C. (1977). Toward a model of the legal doctrine of informed consent. American Journal of Psychiatry, 134 (3), 285–289. Stanley, B., & Galietta, M. (2006). Informed consent in treatment and research. In I. Weiner & A. Hess (Eds.), The handbook of forensic psychology (3rd ed., pp. 211–239). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. 150 ———Consent to Clinical Research C-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:41 PM Page 150 Stanley, B., & Guido, J. (1996). Informed consent: Psychological and empirical issues. In B. H. Stanley, J. E. Sieber, & G. B. Melton (Eds.), Research ethics: A psychological approach (pp. 105–128). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Government Printing Office. (1949). Trials of war criminals before the Nuremberg Military Tribunals under Control Council Law (No. 10, Vol. 2, pp. 181–182). Washington, DC: Author. C OPING S TRATEGIES OF ADULT S EXUAL A SSAULT V ICTIMS Coping strategies of adult rape victims refers to the ways in which rape victims respond to an assault. Most of this research focuses on female victims because most sexual assault victims are women. Although the term coping implies that someone is adjusting well, coping strategies can actually be either helpful or unhelpful. Less helpful strategies include avoiding dealing with the assault, withdrawing from others, using drugs and alcohol to self-medicate, blaming one’s self, and focus- ing on the past and on why the assault occurred. Among the more helpful strategies reported are seeing the assault in a more positive light, relying on one’s reli- gious beliefs, and focusing on what currently is control- lable. Disclosing the assault can lead to either positive or negative reactions from others. Positive reactions are more common, although negative reactions have a greater impact. Knowledge about the strategies victims use to deal with an assault is relevant to several kinds of profes- sionals who work with victims. For example, knowl- edge about which strategies have been helpful or unhelpful is useful to those who provide direct ser- vices to victims (e.g., psychologists, social workers, advocates). This information also might be used by a psychological expert in a sexual assault case to help explain the effects of the rape on a victim to a judge or jury. It is important, however, to keep in mind that most rapes are not reported and, of those that are reported, many are not charged and few go to trial. Specific Coping Strategies In the general research literature on coping, nine primary forms of coping have been identified: cogni- tive restructuring, problem solving, support seeking, distraction, avoidance, social withdrawal, emotional regulation/expressing emotions, rumination, and help- lessness. Several studies have assessed the frequency with which rape victims use these strategies and the relationship between the use of the strategy and the postrape symptoms of distress. Typical symptoms of distress among rape victims include those associated with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), depres- sion, and anxiety. Of the studies that have assessed one of these forms of coping among rape victims, avoidance and social withdrawal are generally associ- ated with greater distress. Avoidance involves trying not to think about or deal with the assault (e.g., trying to block it out), and social withdrawal, of course, involves withdrawing from others. The strategies associated with better adjustment fall mainly within the category of cognitive restructuring, which refers to trying to see the assault in a different or more positive light. Although this might seem unlikely, many vic- tims do actually report positive changes in their lives following an assault, such as that they appreciate life more. Teaching coping skills such as cognitive restructuring also has been found to reduce symptoms in experimental studies. The results of studies assess- ing emotional regulation/expressing emotions are mixed, partly because of methodological problems with the studies. However, the general research litera- ture on coping suggests that expressing emotions is associated with better adjustment. Another coping strategy not included in this categorization scheme is religious coping, which generally is associated with lower distress levels among victims. Another way in which victims may try to cope with the assault is by drinking or using drugs. Several studies indicate that victims report more alcohol and drug abuse and dependence than do nonvictims. Because drinking and drug use are risk factors for sex- ual assault, it is important to try to determine if the substance use came before or after the assault. Studies that attempt to assess the timing of the substance abuse relative to the sexual assault generally indicate that the substance abuse started after the assault. Alcohol and drug use by victims is related to higher distress levels; thus, victims may be using substances to self-medicate their distress. Social Support Although support seeking is considered one of the pri- mary forms of coping, research on support processes in Coping Strategies of Adult Sexual Assault Victims ———151 C-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:41 PM Page 151 rape victims goes beyond the examination of support seeking as a coping process. For example, one issue for rape victims is whether and to whom to disclose the assault. Although disclosing the assault may not neces- sarily be for the purpose of seeking support, unlike other events that are more public (e.g., bereavement), victims only receive supportive or unsupportive reac- tions from others if they disclose the assault to them. The act of disclosing the assault or of seeking support also should be distinguished from the supportive or unsupportive reactions of others to the victim regarding the assault. Thus, research in this area has focused on whether and to whom victims disclose, what kinds of reactions they receive, and how those reactions are related to victims’ distress levels. This research sug- gests that although most rape victims do not report the assault to the police, most disclose to someone, such as friends or family members. In general, victims indicate that they received mostly positive and supportive reac- tions from others. However, negative reactions, such as being blamed or treated differently, also occur and appear to be more common from formal (e.g., the police, physicians) than from informal (e.g., friends, family) support providers. Negative social reactions tend to be more associated with distress than positive social reactions are associated with well-being. Nonethe- less, being believed and being listened to by others, especially friends and family, is associated with better adjustment among rape victims. Many friends and vic- tims report that the assault had a positive effect on their relationships (e.g., it brought them closer). Attributions About the Cause of the Rape Individuals who have been sexually assaulted often struggle to regain a sense of control over their lives. One way to do this is to try to identify the cause of the assault (i.e., make a causal attribution) and thus recog- nize how the assault could have been prevented. For rape victims, this may involve trying to identify what they could have done differently to avoid being raped. Much of the research on the relations among attribu- tions and posttrauma distress has been guided by the theory that behavioral self-blame, which involves attributing the assault to one’s past behavior, is an adaptive response to rape because it is associated with the belief that future rapes can be avoided. In contrast, characterological self-blame, which involves attribut- ing the rape to some stable aspect of one’s self that cannot be changed, is thought to be maladaptive because it is not associated with a sense of future con- trol. This theory has been described as dominating research on attributions and adjustment for more than two decades and as being widely accepted as having implications for interventions with trauma survivors. Although this theory suggests that behavioral self- blame is adaptive, behavioral self-blame consistently is associated with more, rather than less, distress among survivors of rape. In addition, behavioral self- blame generally is unrelated to perceived future con- trol among victims of rape. In other words, behavioral self-blame does not appear to foster the belief that future rapes can be avoided, which was the proposed mechanism for its adaptive value. Characterological self-blame is also consistently related to higher distress levels. Indeed, the two types of self-blame are highly correlated with each other. Experimental studies sug- gest that reductions in self-blame in treatment are asso- ciated with reductions in PTSD symptoms. Research on attributions has tended to focus on self- blame, perhaps because of the predominance of the aforementioned theory. However, other kinds of blame, such as blaming the rapist or other external factors, tend to be more common than self-blame. These other types of blame also are associated with more distress. It appears that focusing on the past and on why the assault occurred is associated with higher distress levels. It is less adaptive to focus on the past and on why the assault occurred, or even on how future assaults can be avoided, than to focus on aspects of the assault that are currently controllable, such as the recovery process. Patricia A. Frazier See also Child Sexual Abuse; Rape Trauma Syndrome; Victimization Further Readings Filipas, H., & Ullman, S. (2001). Social reactions to sexual assault victims from various support sources. Violence and Victims, 16, 673–692. Frazier, P. (2003). Perceived control and distress following sexual assault: A longitudinal test of a new model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1257–1269. Frazier, P., Mortensen, H., & Steward, J. (2005). Coping strategies as mediators of the relations among perceived control and distress in sexual assault survivors. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52, 267–278. 152 ———Coping Strategies of Adult Sexual Assault Victims C-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:41 PM Page 152 Ullman, S. (2000). Psychometric characteristics of the Social Reactions Questionnaire: A measure of reactions to sexual assault victims. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24, 257–271. Ullman, S., Filipas, H., Townsend, S., & Starzynski, L. (2005). Trauma exposure, posttraumatic stress disorder and problem drinking in sexual assault survivors. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 66, 610–619. C RIMINAL B EHAVIOR , THEORIES OF When crime is truly the product of rational choice, the offender commits the act for reasons of personal gain or gratification. His or her behavior is under his or her complete control. How and to what degree, however, might other factors intrude on and compromise his or her ability to exercise free will? The response to this question has come in the form of innumerable theories, each purporting to explain criminal behavior in terms of specific factors. Broadly speaking, these theories involve three categories of factors: psychological, bio- logical, and social. In fact, human behavior is the prod- uct of complex interactions among many factors. Rather than providing a summary of myriad theories, this entry focuses on the main factors involved in the expression and suppression of criminal behaviors. Social Factors There is a vast criminological literature that identifies a wide range of environmental factors as causally linked to criminal behavior. These include develop- mental, social, and economic factors. For example, poverty is often cited as a socioeconomic condition linked to crime. The stress, strain, and frustration experienced by those lacking the financial resources to meet their needs and fulfill their desires through legitimate means renders them more inclined to com- mit crime than affluent individuals with ready access to legitimate means. Poor nutrition is an especially troubling aspect of poverty. Nutritional deficiencies can result in or exacerbate problems such as learning disabilities and poor impulse control. Such cognitive dysfunctions have been identified as precursors to delinquency and adult criminality. Thus, one’s position in the social structure of society—as opera- tionalized by variables such as level of income—can be a significant contributing factor in the criminal activities of some individuals by virtue of its impact on brain function. Growing up in a household where parental displays of violence are commonplace can shape the behavior of children so as to make them more likely to respond to their own problems with violent means. While aggression and violence are not synonymous, that they are correlated is undeniable. Psychologist Albert Bandura demonstrated the importance of social learn- ing in the development of aggressive behavior. Exposure to a violent role model may operate as a trigger of preexisting psychological and biological factors that predispose that individual to aggressive behavior. This may explain why only one of the two sons whose father assaults their mother grows up to beat his own wife—there were additional factors that rendered him more readily influenced by the violent model; or, conversely, the nonviolent son was resistant to the influence by virtue of individual “protective” factors, such as high IQ. There is a substantial literature on a “cycle of vio- lence” whereby victims of childhood abuse and neglect are predisposed to engage in violent behavior in adulthood, thus passing the violence from one gen- eration to the next. Other research has examined the effects of being bullied during formative years, find- ing that the victims in turn become victimizers. In ani- mal experiments, exposure to conditions of inescapable threat has been found to alter specific chemicals in the brain involved in aggression and the inhibition of aggression, with the result that formerly docile ani- mals go on to display inappropriate and excessive aggression, attacking smaller, weaker animals whenever presented with them. In essence, they become the “pl ayground bully.” Thus, a change in the environ ment—exposure to inescapable threat— leads to changes in biology, which lead to the changes in behavior. Empirical studies on the effects of child maltreatment reveal that in addition to ps