t to which clinicians separately consider and differentially interpret PCL factor scores; and so forth. Another issue concerning the assessment of psychopathy that is important in clinical practice but that has received little research attention is the ability of those being evaluated to intentionally influence or manipulate their PCL scores. The impetus to present oneself in a particular way would seem to be considerable in forensic contexts. The public has easy ac- cess via popular books (e.g., Hare, 1993) to detailed accounts of the procedures used to assess psychopathy and to descrip- tions of the key symptoms of psychopathy substantively sim- ilar to the criteria outlined in the PCL-R manual. Given that PCL assessments are based on lifetime functioning and rely heavily on collateral sources, it seems unlikely that PCL scores could be markedly distorted. Research might nonethe- less clarify the parameters under which PCL scores could be distorted (e.g., when collateral information is limited) and the PCL items most susceptible to distortion. Evaluate Treatment Efficacy It makes good sense to believe that psychopaths will change little as a consequence of treatment or other interventions (at least, not in the short term). Psychopaths, by definition, expe- rience little remorse or guilt that might propel them into treatment. They are not motivated to actively participate in treatment once enrolled because they see little wrong with themselves, they lack insight and do not recognize the adverse impact that their behaviors have on others, and they habitually lie and manipulate others. These characteristics are generally the antithesis of those that have been found to be important for effecting positive therapeutic change. Many readers may be surprised, therefore, to learn that virtu- ally no methodologically sound treatment study has been conducted evaluating the treatment efficacy of a contem- porary treatment program for psychopaths. Most of the evi- dence concerning poor treatment outcomes ascribed to criminal psychopaths is based on anecdotal case studies or weak research designs (e.g., see Dolan & Coid, 1993; Hemphill & Hart, in press; Wong & Elek, 1989; Wong & Hare, in press). Perhaps the most methodologically rigorous and oft-cited research study to date concerning the efficacy of treatment for psychopaths was conducted by Rice et al. (1992). These authors concluded that treated psychopaths were more violent than were untreated psychopaths during a 10.5-year follow-up. It is important to recognize that this treatment program, although considered innovative in the late 1960s and 1970s, is a nontraditional treatment program that “would not meet current ethical standards” (Harris, Rice, & Cormier, 1991; p. 628). Research that evaluates the efficacy of treatment among psychopaths and that addresses a number of basic method- ological concerns is clearly a priority. Methodologically su- perior studies would include large groups of clearly defined psychopaths who have received well-established treatments that have been delivered consistently and evaluated systemat- ically across long follow-up periods using several measures of treatment outcome. Although research methodologies have improved greatly across time (e.g., Hare et al., 2000; Hobson, Shine, & Roberts, 2000; Seto & Barbaree, 1999), there is still considerable room for improvement concerning studies that examine the efficacy of treatment among offend- ers in general and among psychopaths in particular. SUMMARY The procedures for assessing psychopathy can be grouped into three broad categories: structured diagnostic inter- views; self-report questionnaires and inventories; and ex- pert rating scales. This chapter critically examined each of these three broad procedures while keeping in mind the unique assessment issues with respect to forensic contexts and psychopathy assessments. Expert rating systems are considered superior to the other two categories for assess- ing psychopathy. A variety of professional and clinical is- sues that clinicians should keep in mind when conducting psychopathy assessments were discussed, as were practical recommendations for dealing with many of these issues. The chapter concluded with an examination of inadequately studied areas concerning psychopathy that should be a focus of future research. 104 Forensic and Clinical Issues in the Assessment of Psychopathy REFERENCES American Educational Research Association, American Psycholog- ical Association, & National Council on Measurement in Educa- tion. (1999). Standards for educational and psychological testing. Washington, DC: American Educational Research Asso- ciation. American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statisti- cal manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statisti- cal manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Arieti, S. (1963). Psychopathic personality: Some views on its psy- chopathology and psychodynamics. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 4 , 301–312. Barry, C. T., Frick, P. J., DeShazo, T. M., McCoy, M. G., Ellis, M., & Loney, B. R. (2000). The importance of callous-unemotional traits for extending the concept of psychopathy to children. Jour- nal of Abnormal Psychology, 109 , 335–340. Bayer, B. M., Bonta, J. L., & Motiuk, L. L. (1985). The Pd sub- scales: An empirical evaluation. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41 , 780–788. Blackburn, R., & Fawcett, D. (1999). The Antisocial Personality Questionnaire: An inventory for assessing personality deviation in offender populations. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 15 , 14–24. Butcher, J. N., Dahlstrom, L., Graham, J. R., Tellgen,A., & Kaemmer, B. (1989). Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory–2. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Cleckley, H. (1941). The mask of sanity . St. Louis, MO: Mosby. Committee on Ethical Guidelines for Forensic Psychologists. (1991). Specialty guidelines for forensic psychologists. Law and Human Behavior, 15 , 655–665. Cooke, D. J. (1996). Psychopathic personality in different cultures: What do we know? What do we need to find out? Journal of Personality Disorders, 10 , 23–40. Cooke, D. J., Forth, A. E., & Hare, R. D. (Eds.). (1998). Psychopa- thy: Theory, research, and implications for society . Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Press. Cooke, D. J., Kosson, D. S., & Michie, C. (2001). Psychopathy and ethnicity: Structural, item and test generalizability of the Psy- chopathy Checklist–Revised in Caucasian and African-American participants . Manuscript in preparation. Cooke, D. J., & Michie, C. (1997). An item response theory analy- sis of the Hare Psychopathy Checklist–Revised. Psychological Assessment, 9 , 3–14. Cooke, D. J., & Michie, C. (1999). Psychopathy across cultures: Scotland and North America compared. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108 , 58–68. Cooke, D. J., & Michie, C. (2001). Refining the construct of psychopathy: Toward a hierarchical model. Psychological Assessment, 13 , 171–188. Cooke, D. J., Michie, C., Hart, S. D., & Hare, R. D. (1999). Evalu- ating the screening version of the Hare Psychopathy Checklist–Revised (PCL:SV): An item response theory analysis. Psychological Assessment, 11, 3–13. Cooney, N. L., Kadden, R. M., & Litt, M. D. (1990). A comparison of methods for assessing sociopathy in male and female alco- holics. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 51, 42–48. Cunningham, M. D., & Reidy, T. J. (1998). Antisocial personality disorder and psychopathy: Diagnostic dilemmas in classifying patterns of antisocial behavior in sentencing evaluations. Behav- ioral Sciences and the Law, 16, 333–351. Cunningham, M. D., & Reidy, T. J. (1999). Don’t confuse me with the facts: Common errors in violence risk assessment at capital sentencing. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 26, 20–43. Darke, S., Kaye, S., Finlay-Jones, R., & Hall, W. (1998). Factor structure of psychopathy among methadone maintenance pa- tients. Journal of Personality Disorders, 12, 162–171. Davies, W., & Feldman, P. (1981). The diagnosis of psychopathy by forensic specialists. British Journal of Psychiatry, 138, 329–331. Dolan, B., & Coid, J. (1993). Psychopathic and antisocial personal- ity disorders: Treatment and research issues. London: Gaskell. Edens, J. F., Hart, S. D., Johnson, D. W., Johnson, J. K., & Olver, M. E. (2000). Use of the Personality Assessment Inventory to assess psychopathy in offender populations. Psychological Assessment, 12, 132–139. Edens, J. F., Poythress, N. G., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (1999). Identify- ing inmates at risk for disciplinary infractions: A comparison of two measures of psychopathy. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 17, 435–443. Edens, J. F., Skeem, J. L., Cruise, K. R., & Cauffman, E. (2001). Assessment of “juvenile psychopathy” and its association with violence: A critical review. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 19, 53–80. Feighner, J. P., Robins, E., Guze, S. B., Woodruff, R. A., Winokur, G., & Munoz, R. (1972). Diagnostic criteria for use in psychi- atric research. Archives of General Psychiatry, 26, 57–63. First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., Williams, J. B. W., Davies, M., Borus, J., et al. (1995). The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R personality disorders (SCID-II) II: Multi-site test- retest reliability study. Journal of Personality Disorders, 9, 92–104. Forth, A. E., & Burke, H. C. (1998). Psychopathy in adolescence: Assessment, violence, and developmental precursors. In D. J. Cooke, A. E. Forth, & R. D. Hare (Eds.), Psychopathy: Theory, research, and implications for society (pp. 205–229). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Press. Forth, A. E., Hart, S. D., & Hare, R. D. (1990). Assessment of psy- chopathy in male young offenders. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 2, 342–344. References 105 Frick, P. J., & Ellis, M. (1999). Callous-unemotional traits and sub- types of conduct disorder. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 2, 149–168. Graham, J. R. (1999). MMPI-2: Assessing personality and psy- chopathology (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. Gresham, F. M., Lane, K. L., & Lambros, K. M. (2000). Comorbid- ity of conduct problems and ADHD: Identification of fledgling psychopaths. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 8, 83–93. Gustaffson, S. B., & Ritzer, D. R. (1995). The dark side of normal: A psychopathy-linked pattern called aberrant self-promotion. European Journal of Personality, 9, 1–37. Hare, R. D. (1980). A research scale for the assessment of psy- chopathy in criminal populations. Personality and Individual Differences, 1, 111–119. Hare, R. D. (1983). Diagnosis of antisocial personality disorder in two prison populations. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 887–890. Hare, R. D. (1985). A comparison of procedures for the assessment of psychopathy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 7–16. Hare, R. D. (1991). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist–Revised. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health Systems. Hare, R. D. (1993). Without conscience: The disturbing world of the psychopaths among us . New York: Pocket Books. Hare, R. D. (1996). Psychopathy: A clinical construct whose time has come. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 23, 25–54. Hare, R. D. (1998). The Hare PCL-R: Some issues concerning its use and misuse. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 3, 99–119. Hare, R. D., Clark, D., Grann, M., & Thornton, D. (2000). Psy- chopathy and the predictive validity of the PCL-R: An interna- tional perspective. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 18, 623–645. Hare, R. D., Cooke, D. J., & Hart, S. D. (1999). Psychopathy and sadistic personality disorder. In T. Millon, P. H. Blaney, & R. D. Davis (Eds.), Oxford textbook of psychopathology (pp. 555–584). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Hare, R. D., Harpur, T. J., Hakstian, A. R., Forth, A. E., Hart, S. D., & Newman, J. P. (1990). The Revised Psychopathy Checklist: Reliability and factor structure. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 2, 338–341. Hare, R. D., Hart, S. D., & Harpur, T. J. (1991). Psychopathy and the DSM-IV criteria for antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 391–398. Harpur, T. J., Hakstian, R. A., & Hare, R. D. (1988). Factor structure of the Psychopathy Checklist. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 741–747. Harpur, T. J., Hare, R. D., & Hakstian, R. A. (1989). Two-factor con- ceptualization of psychopathy: Construct validity and assess- ment implications. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1, 6–17. Harris, G. T., Rice, M. E., & Cormier, C. A. (1991). Psychopathy and violent recidivism. Law and Human Behavior, 15, 625–637. Harris, G. T., Rice, M. E., & Quinsey, V. L. (1993). Violent recidi- vism of mentally disordered offenders: The development of a statistical prediction instrument. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 20, 315–335. Harris, G. T., Rice, M. E., & Quinsey, V. L. (1994). Psychopathy as a taxon: Evidence that psychopaths are a discrete class. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 387–397. Hart, S. D. (1998). The role of psychopathy in assessing risk for violence: Conceptual and methodological issues. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 3, 121–137. Hart, S. D. (2001). Forensic issues. In W. J. Livesley (Ed.), Hand- book of personality disorders: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 555–569). New York: Guilford Press. Hart, S. D., Cox, D. N., & Hare, R. D. (1995). Manual for the Hare Psychopathy Checklist–Revised: Screening version (PCL:SV). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health Systems. Hart, S. D., Forth, A. E., & Hare, R. D. (1991). The MCMI-II as a measure of psychopathy. Journal of Personality Disorders, 5, 318–327. Hart, S. D., & Hare, R. D. (1989). Discriminant validity of the Psychopathy Checklist in a forensic psychiatric population. Psy- chological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1, 211–218. Hart, S. D., & Hare, R. D. (1997). Psychopathy: Assessment and association with criminal conduct. In D. M. Stoff, J. Breiling, & J. D. Maser (Eds.), Handbook of antisocial behavior (pp. 22–35). New York: Wiley. Hathaway, S. R., & McKinley, J. C. (1940). A multiphasic personal- ity schedule (Minnesota). I: Construction of the schedule. Jour- nal of Psychology, 10, 249–254. Haynes, S. N., Richard, D. C. S., & Kubany, E. S. (1995). Content validity in psychological assessment: A functional approach to concepts and methods. Psychological Assessment, 7, 238–247. Heilbrun, K. (1992). The role of psychological testing in forensic as- sessment. Law and Human Behavior, 16, 257–272. Hemphill, J. F., Hare, R. D., & Wong, S. (1998). Psychopathy and recidivism: A review. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 3, 139–170. Hemphill, J. F. & Hart, S. D. (in press). Motivating the unmotivated: Psychopathy, treatment, and change. In M. McMurran (Ed.), Motivating offenders to change: A guide to enhancing engage- ment in therapy. Chichester, England: Wiley. Hemphill, J. F., Hart, S. D., & Hare, R. D. (1994). Psychopathy and substance use. Journal of Personality Disorders, 8, 169–180. Hemphill, J. F., Newman, J. P., & Hare, R. D. (2001, April). Psy- chopathy and recidivism among Black and White adult male of- fenders . Paper presented at the International Association of Forensic Mental Health Services, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada. 106 Forensic and Clinical Issues in the Assessment of Psychopathy Hemphill, J. F., Templeman, R., Wong, S., & Hare, R. D. (1998). Psychopathy and crime: Recidivism and criminal careers. In D. J. Cooke, A. E. Forth, & R. D. Hare (Eds.), Psychopathy: The- ory, research and implications for society (pp. 375–399). Dor- drecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Press. Henard, D. H. (2000). Item response theory. In L. G. Grimm & P. R. Yarnold (Eds.), Reading and understanding more multivariate statistics (pp. 67–97). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Hobson, J., Shine, J., & Roberts, R. (2000). How do psychopaths behave in a prison therapeutic community? Psychology, Crime and Law, 6, 139–154. Janus, E. S. (2000). Sexual predator commitment laws: Lessons for law and the behavioral sciences. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 18, 5–21. Karpman, B. (1961). The structure of neuroses: With special differ- entials between neurosis, psychosis, homosexuality, alcoholism, psychopathy and criminality. Archives of Criminal Psychody- namics, 4, 599–646. Kosson, D. S., Smith, S. S., & Newman, J. P. (1990). Evaluation of the construct validity of psychopathy in Black and White male inmates: Three preliminary studies. Journal of Abnormal Psy- chology, 99, 250–259. Levenson, M. R., Kiehl, K. A., & Fitzpatrick, C. M. (1995). Assess- ing psychopathic attributes in a non-institutionalized population. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 151–158. Lilienfeld, S. O. (1994). Conceptual problems in the assessment of psychopathy. Clinical Psychology Review, 14, 17–38. Lilienfeld, S. O., & Andrews, B. P. (1996). Development and pre- liminary validation of a self report measure of psychopathic per- sonality traits in noncriminal populations. Journal of Personality Assessment, 66, 488–524. Loranger, A. W., Sartorius, N., Andreoli, A., Berger, P., Buchheim, P., Channabasavanna, S. M., et al. (1994). The International Personality Disorder Examination: The World Health Organiza- tion/Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration international pilot study of personality disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 215–224. Lynam, D. R. (1996). Early identification of chronic offenders: Who is the fledgling psychopath? Psychological Bulletin, 120, 209–234. Lynam, D. R. (1997). Pursuing the psychopath: Capturing the fledg- ling psychopath in a nomological net. Journal of Abnormal Behavior, 106, 425–438. Lyon, D. R., & Ogloff, J. R. P. (2000). Legal and ethical issues in psychopathy assessment. In C. B. Gacono (Ed.), The clinical and forensic assessment of psychopathy: A practitioner’s guide (pp. 139–173). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. McCord, W., & McCord, J. (1964). The psychopath: An essay on the criminal mind . Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand. McDermott, P. A., Alterman, A. I., Cacciola, J. S., Rutherford, M. J., Newman, J. P., & Mulholland, E. M. (2000). Generality of Psychopathy Checklist–Revised factors over prisoners and substance-dependent patients. Journal of Consulting and Clini- cal Psychology, 68, 181–186. McKinley, J. C., & Hathaway, S. R. (1944). The Minnesota Multi- phasic Personality Inventory V: Hysteria, hypomania, and psy- chopathic deviate. Journal of Applied Psychology, 28, 153–174. Melton, G. B., Petrila, J., Poythress, N. G., & Slobogin, C. (1997). Psychological evaluations for the courts: A handbook for mental health professionals and lawyers (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. Millon, T. (1987). Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory. II: Manual . Minneapolis, MN: National Computer Systems. Millon, T., Davis, R., & Millon, C. (1997). Millon Clinical Multiax- ial Inventory–III (MCMI-III) manual (2nd ed.). Minneapolis, MN: National Computer Systems. Morey, L. C. (1991). The Personality Assessment Inventory pro- fessional manual . Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Neighbors, H. (1987). The prevalence of mental disorder in Michigan prisons. DIS Newsletter, 4, 8–11. Ogloff, J. R. P. (1995). Navigating the quagmire: Legal and ethical considerations. In D. G. Martin & A. D. Moore (Eds.), First steps in the art of intervention: A guidebook for trainees in the helping professions (pp. 347–376). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. Ogloff, J. R. P., & Lyon, D. R. (1998). Legal issues associated with the concept of psychopathy. In D. J. Cooke, A. E. Forth, & R. D. Hare (Eds.), Psychopathy: Theory, research, and implications for soci- ety (pp. 401–422). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Press. Pope, K. S., Butcher, J. N., & Seelen, J. (1993). The MMPI, MMPI-2, and MMPI-A in court: A practical guide for expert witnesses and attorneys . Washington, DC: American Psycho- logical Association. Porter, S., Fairweather, D., Drugge, J., Hervé, H., Birt, A., & Boer, D. P. (2000). Profiles of psychopathy in incarcerated sexual offenders. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 27, 216–233. Rice, M. E., & Harris, G. T. (1997). Cross-validation and extension of the Violence Risk Appraisal Guide for child molesters and rapists. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 231–241. Rice, M. E., Harris, G. T., & Cormier, C. A. (1992). An evaluation of a maximum security therapeutic community for psychopaths and other mentally disordered offenders. Law and Human Behavior, 16, 399–412. Robins, L. N. (1966). Deviant children grown up: A sociological and psychiatric study of sociopathic personality . Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. Robins, L. N., Tipp, J., & Przybeck, T. (1991). Antisocial personality. In L. N. Robins & D. Regier (Eds.), Psychiatric dis- orders in America: The Epidemiologic Catchment Area study (pp. 258–290). New York: Free Press. Rogers, R. (1995). Diagnostic and structured interviewing: A hand- book for psychologists . Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. References 107 Rogers, R., & Dion, K. (1991). Rethinking the DSM-III-R diagnosis of antisocial personality disorder . Bulletin of the American Academy of Psychiatry and Law, 19, 21–31. Rogers, R., Dion, K. L., & Lynett, E. (1992). Diagnostic validity of antisocial personality disorder: A prototypical analysis. Law and Human Behavior, 16, 677–689. Rogers, R., Duncan, J. C., Lynett, E., & Sewell, K. W. (1994). Pro- totypical analysis of antisocial personality disorder: DSM-IV and beyond. Law and Human Behavior, 18, 471–484. Rogers, R., Gillis, J. R., & Dickens, S. E. (1989). A research note on the MMPI Pd scale and sociopathy. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 33, 21–25. Rutherford, M. J., Cacciola, J. S., Alterman, A. I., & McKay, J. R. (1996). Reliability and validity of the Revised Psychopathy Checklist in women methadone patients. Assessment, 3, 145–156. Rutherford, M., Cacciola, J. S., Alterman, A. I., McKay, J. R., & Cook, T. G. (1999). The 2-year test-retest reliability of the Psy- chopathy Checklist–Revised in methadone patients. Assessment, 6, 285–291. Salekin, R., Rogers, R., & Sewell, K. (1996). A review and meta- analysis of the Psychopathy Checklist and Psychopathy Checklist–Revised: Predictive validity of dangerousness. Clin- ical Psychology: Science and Practice, 3, 203–215. Salekin, R. T., Rogers, R., & Sewell, K. W. (1997). Construct valid- ity of psychopathy in a female offender sample: A multitrait- multimethod evaluation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 576–585. Schroeder, M. L., Schroeder, K. G., & Hare, R. D. (1983). General- izability of a checklist for assessment of psychopathy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 511–516. Schuller, R. A., & Ogloff, J. R. P. (2001). An introduction to psychol- ogy and law. In R. A. Schuller & J. R. P. Ogloff (Eds.), Introduc- tion to psychology and law: Canadian perspectives (pp. 3–28). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: University of Toronto Press. Sechrest, L. (1963). Incremental validity: A recommendation. Edu- cational and Psychological Measurement, 23, 153–158. Seto, M. C., & Barbaree, H. E. (1999). Psychopathy, treatment be- havior, and sex offender recidivism. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 14, 1235–1248. Steadman, H. J., Silver, E., Monahan, J., Appelbaum, P. S., Robbins, P. C., Mulvey, E. P., et al. (2000). A classification tree approach to the development of actuarial violence risk assessment tools. Law and Human Behavior, 24, 83–100. Stuart, S., Pfhol, B., Battaglia, M., Bellodi, L., Grove, W., & Cadoret, R. (1998). The co-occurrence of DSM-III-R personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 12, 302–315. Tennent, G., Tennent, D., Prins, H., & Bedford, A. (1990). Psycho- pathic disorder: A useful clinical concept? Medicine, Science, and the Law, 30, 39–44. Trestman, R. L. (2000). Behind bars: Personality disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 28, 232–235. Vincent, G., & Hart, S. D. (in press). Psychopathy in adolescents. In R. R. Corrado, R. Roesch, S. D. Hart, & J. Gierowski, J. (Eds.), Multi-problem violent youth: A foundation for comparative re- search on needs, interventions, and outcomes . Amsterdam: IOS Press. Werlinder, H. (1978). Psychopathy: A history of the concepts. Analysis of the origin and development of a family of concepts in psychopathology . Doctoral dissertation, Institute of Education, Uppsala University, Sweden. Widiger, T. A., Cadoret, R., Hare, R. D., Robins, L., Rutherford, M., Zanarini, M., et al. (1996). DSM-IV antisocial personality disorder field trial. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 3–16. Widiger, T. A., & Corbitt, E. M. (1993). Antisocial personality dis- order: Proposals for DSM-IV. Journal of Personality Disorders, 7, 63–77. Widiger, T. A., & Corbitt, E. M. (1995). Antisocial personality dis- order in DSM-IV . In J. Livesley (Ed.), DSM-IV personality disorders (pp. 127–134). New York: Guilford Press. Widiger, T. A., & Sanderson, C. J. (1995). Toward a dimensional model of personality disorders. In W. J. Livesley (Ed.), The DSM-IV personality disorders (pp. 433–458). New York: Guilford Press. Wiggins, J. S. (1973). Personality and prediction: Principles of personality assessment . Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Wilson, J. Q., & Herrnstein, R. J. (1985). Crime and human nature . New York: Touchstone. Wong, S., & Elek, D. (1989). The treatment of psychopathy: A re- view . Unpublished manuscript. Wong, S., & Hare, R. D. (in press). Program guidelines for the in- stitutional treatment of violent psychopaths . Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health Systems. World Health Organization. (1992). ICD-10: International statisti- cal classification of diseases and related health problems (10th rev.). Geneva, Switzerland: Author. Zimmerman, M. (1994). Diagnosing personality disorders: A review of issues and research methods. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 225–245. Zinger, I., & Forth, A. (1998). Psychopathy and Canadian criminal proceedings: The potential for human rights abuses. Canadian Journal of Criminology, 40, 237–276. CHAPTER 7 Evaluation of Malingering and Deception RICHARD ROGERS AND SCOTT D. BENDER 109 CONCEPTUAL ISSUES 109 Definitions of Response Styles 109 Perspectives of Malingering in the Forensic Context 110 Explanatory Models of Malingering 110 Explanatory Models of Defensiveness 111 Misassumptions about Malingering and Dissimulation 111 Applications to Forensic Practice 112 EMPIRICAL ISSUES 112 Basic Designs 112 Incremental Validity 113 MALINGERING AND MENTAL DISORDERS 114 Detection Strategies 114 Featured Measures 114 DEFENSIVENESS AND MENTAL DISORDERS 118 Overview 118 Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory 118 Paulhus Deception Scales 119 FEIGNED COGNITIVE IMPAIRMENT 119 Detection Strategies 120 Guidelines for the Classification 123 Featured Measures 124 SUMMARY 126 REFERENCES 126 CONCEPTUAL ISSUES The validity of most psychological measures is predicated on the cardinal assumption that evaluatees are responding in a forthright manner and putting forth a sincere effort. Is this as- sumption warranted in forensic practice? External influences on self-reporting and effort may include the adversarial effects of litigation and pressures exerted by interested others, such as attorneys and family members. Internal influences may in- clude (a) reactions to questioned credibility, (b) stigmatization of mental disorders or disability status, © effects of a genuine disorder, or (d) efforts to obtain undeserved benefits. Forensic psychologists tend to focus on the last as it relates to malinger- ing and de-emphasize other internal and external influences. Forensic psychologists may wish to address openly internal and external influences that potentially arise from their evalua- tions.As part of the informed consent process, they may choose to ask evaluatees about their understanding of the purposes of the evaluation and what they have been told about the evalua- tion by others. Disclosures from the forensic psychologist about the purpose of the evaluation and his or her role may allay some concerns about partiality. Especially in civil cases, an un- hurried and respectful discussion of the evaluation, its purpose, and parameters is needed to address strong negative reactions regarding perceived coercion (e.g., “I had to come”) or ques- tioned legitimacy (e.g., “You think I am making this up”). Tests of cognitive abilities and achievement are premised on optimal effort by evaluatees. Less than optimal effort may vitiate the accuracy of test results and lead to concerns about deliberate underperformance. A largely neglected considera- tion is the effect of genuine disorders on test performance. For example, major depression may reduce performance on cognitive tasks that require sustained attention and concen- tration. Forensic psychologists are cautioned against facile and unwarranted assumptions that suboptimal efforts are always equated with malingering. This section provides an overview of response styles with a summary of accepted terminology. Three general perspec- tives of malingering are explicated. Explanatory models are reviewed with a discussion of inferred motivations for why persons engage in malingering and defensiveness. In addi- tion, misassumptions about response styles are examined in the context of forensic evaluations. Definitions of Response Styles Rogers (1997) summarized the basic terminology used to de- scribe response styles. Basic definitions are provided with several updated references: • Malingering (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) is the deliberate fabrication or gross exaggeration of 110 Evaluation of Malingering and Deception psychological or physical symptoms for the fulfillment of an external goal. • Defensiveness is the polar opposite of malingering; it is the deliberate denial or gross minimization of symptoms in the service of an external goal. • Irrelevant responding is a disengagement from the assess- ment process typically reflected in inconsistent responding that is unrelated to the specific content (e.g., not reading test items). • Feigning is the deliberate fabrication or gross exaggera- tion of psychological or physical symptoms (Rogers & Vitacco, in press) without any assumptions about its goals. Available tests typically assess feigning, because they are unable to evaluate supposed goals required for the classification of malingering or the diagnosis of factitious disorders. • Secondary gain is an imprecise clinical term that should be avoided in forensic evaluations (Rogers & Reinhardt, 1998). In nonforensic settings, the term is used to describe the perpetuation and possible augmentation of symptoms based on unintentional responses to internal (i.e., psycho- dynamic models) or external (i.e., behavioral-medicine models) forces. • Suboptimal effort (also called “incomplete effort”) is a de- scriptive inference that maximum performance was not achieved. Suboptimal effort may be the result of internal states (e.g., fatigue or frustration) or comorbidity (e.g., de- pression subsequent to a head injury). Only when subopti- mal effort is extreme in its presentation should feigning be considered, although internal states and comorbidity must still be addressed. • Dissimulation is a general term to describe an inaccurate portrayal of symptoms and associated features. It is typi- cally used when more precise terms (e.g., malingering and defensiveness) are inapplicable. Perspectives of Malingering in the Forensic Context A heuristic typology is proposed to explain differences in how forensic psychologists approach the evaluation of response styles. Three main perspectives are identified: intuitional, standard, and specialized. These perspectives are considered in the context of malingering. The intuitional perspective presupposes that malingering and other response styles will be recognizable based on clinical acumen without the need for empirically validated strategies, scales, and indicators. Despite its lack of empirical validation, we suspect that the intuitional perspective is wide- spread in forensic practice. A key example is found with competency to stand trial evaluations. Despite nearly three decades of research on competency evaluations (Rogers, 2001), malingering and related response styles have been virtually ignored. Even the most recent and best-funded competency measure, MacArthur Competency Assessment Tool–Criminal Adjudication (Poythress et al., 1999), implic- itly adopted an intuitional perspective for malingering. While acknowledging that response styles may confound compe- tency evaluations, no indices of any kind are provided (see Poythress et al., 1999, p. 5). The standard perspective routinely evaluates malingering and defensiveness on the basis of traditional tests and mea- sures. The advantages of this approach are twofold: (a) highly efficient use of customary measures for dual purposes (e.g., psychopathology and feigning), and (b) application of empirically tested strategies. The major shortcoming of the standard perspective is that traditional testing lacks the diag- nostic utility for making clinical determinations. The most common examples of the standard perspective involve multiscale inventories (e.g., the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory 2 [MMPI-2; Butcher, Williams, Graham, Tellegen, & Kaemmer, 1989]) and intelligence testing (i.e., predominantly the Wechsler Adult Intelli- gence Scale–Revised [WAIS-R; Weschler, 1981] rather than WAIS-III; Weschler, 1997). The specialized perspective supplements traditional test- ing with measures that are specifically designed for the as- sessment of response styles. Common forensic examples include the Structured Interview of Reported Symptoms (SIRS; Rogers, Bagby, & Dickens, 1992) for feigned mental disorders and the Portland Digit Recognition Test (PDRT; Binder & Willis, 1991) for feigned cognitive im- pairment. Despite the additional expenditure time, the spe- cialized perspective is generally superior to the standard perspective in its classificatory accuracy. The specialized perspective is recommended as the necessary model for the determination of feigning in both clinical and forensic practice. Explanatory Models of Malingering When conducting evaluations and rendering conclusions, forensic psychologists are likely to be influenced by explana- tory models of malingering. Explanatory models attempt to explain why individuals strive to malinger psychological and physical impairment. Rogers (1990a, 1990b) outlined three explanatory models of malingering: pathogenic, crimi- nological, and adaptational. Several prototypical analyses (Rogers, Sewell, & Goldstein, 1994; Rogers, Salekin, Sewell, Goldstein, & Leonard, 1998) provide general support for Conceptual Issues 111 these explanatory models as distinct explanations for malin- gering. A synopsis of the three explanatory models of malin- gering is provided. The pathogenic model assumes that the underlying moti- vation is an ineffective attempt to control the symptoms and clinical presentation of a chronic and progressive men- tal disorder. With increased impairment, intentionally pro- duced symptoms become gradually less deliberate, until they are involuntary and unintended. The pathogenic model predicts that feigning is an ineffectual attempt at adjust- ment that eventually is resolved by the patient’s further deterioration. The criminological model is championed by the Diagnos- tic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders ( DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association, 2000); it assumes that the primary motivation is characterological. Namely, antisocial persons faced with legal difficulties will attempt to garner un- warranted advantages either in circumstances (e.g., a hospital rather than a prison) or material gain (e.g., financial settle- ment). Antisocial persons are presumed to be generally de- ceptive. With malingering viewed as a variant of deception, the criminological model predicts an intermittent use of malingering based on situational opportunities. The adaptational model assumes that the person perceives the circumstances as adversarial and considers malingering to be a feasible alternative. This model avoids the monistic notions of “mad” (pathogenic) or “bad” (criminological) and views malingering in terms of a cost-benefit analysis. The adaptational model views malingering as a situational re- sponse based on an appraisal of alternatives. Rogers, Salekin, et al. (1998) found that the pathogenic model was low in prototypicality for both males and females in forensic evaluations. In contrast, both the adaptational and criminological models achieved moderately high prototypi- cal ratings for forensic cases. A potential danger of the crim- inological model is that forensic psychologists may attempt to use this explanatory model as a detection model. The DSM-IV-TR indices only raise the suspicion of malin- gering; they do not constitute formal criteria for the classifi- cation of malingering. Even for suspicions of malingering, these indices (i.e., antisocial personality disorder, medicole- gal evaluation, uncooperativeness, and results inconsistent with objective findings) falter on both conceptual and empir- ical grounds. Rogers (1997) provides a conceptual analysis of their major shortcomings. Even in defending the DSM-IV- TR indices, LoPiccolo, Goodkin, and Baldewicz (1999) con- ceded most of these shortcomings. Empirically, DSM-IV-TR indices fail entirely even for screening purposes. Their use in a criminal forensic setting resulted in a false-positive rate of approximately 80% (Rogers, 1990a). Explanatory Models of Defensiveness Rogers and Dickey (1991) proposed that explanatory models of defensiveness could be extrapolated from the malingering literature, at least in the case of sex offenders. The pathogenic model is the least persuasive; psychodynamic formulations have suggested that loss of ego functions may result in uncon- scious denial. More persuasive explanations were the crimi- nological and adaptational models, suggesting that denial and gross minimization might result from either a general criminal orientation or an attempt to cope with highly adversarial cir- cumstances. As noted by Rogers and Dickey, sex offenders often are placed in an irresolvable bind: Honesty, disclosing the true extent of their paraphilac behavior, is likely to result in negative sanctions based on the extent of criminal activity; defensiveness, grossly minimizing the true extent of their paraphilac behavior, is likely to result in negative sanctions because nondisclosure is viewed as a barrier to treatment. Sewell and Salekin (1997) expanded on Rogers and Dickey’s (1991) framework and proposed a socioevaluative model of defensiveness. For offenders, especially sex offend- ers, evaluations are consistently linked with punishment and ostracism. The socioevaluative model posits that evaluatees react to the likely threat of a negative outcome and attempt to protect themselves. The socioevaluative model is similar to the adaptational model in its appraisal of a highly adversarial context. It is distinguished from the adaptational model in its generalized reaction. Even when “there is nothing to lose,” the socioevaluative model predicts a generalized response of defensiveness based on past learning. Under the rubric of cognitive distortions, the notion of self-deception has been considered, especially with sex offenders. According to Vanhouche and Vertommen (1999), cognitive distortions involve “learned assumptions” and “sets of beliefs and attitudes” (p. 164) that serve in the denial and minimization of criminal behavior. In the course of the eval- uation, denials of responsibility may be influenced by “self- deceptive” beliefs (e.g., educative goals of incest). However, such denials are unlikely to explain the overall defensiveness expressed by many offenders. The understanding of defensiveness in forensic practice is constrained by the focus on sex offenders. Although extrapo- lations to other forensic populations are possible, explanatory models of defensiveness remain in their initial stages of development and validation. Misassumptions about Malingering and Dissimulation Forensic psychologists are not immune to common misas- sumptions about malingering and other response styles. 112 Evaluation of Malingering and Deception Moreover, forensic psychologists must be prepared to ad- dress erroneous assumptions made by others in the legal sys- tem. Five key misassumptions, common to forensic practice, are outlined: • Malingering is very rare . Equating infrequency with in- consequentiality, some clinicians neglect the evaluation of malingering except in very obvious cases. Estimates (Rogers et al., 1994, 1996) based on more than 500 foren- sic experts suggest that malingering is not rare, but likely occurs in 15% to 17% of forensic cases. • Malingering is very common . Fueled by fears of fraud and injustice, certain attorneys (e.g., defense counsel in civil litigation and prosecutors in criminal matters) suspect that malingering and dissimulation are very prevalent. Despite speculation that the majority of forensic evaluatees may be malingering, the best estimates (Rogers et al., 1994, 1996) indicate this is not the case. • Malingering occurs at a predictable rate . If stable base rates could be achieved, the classification of malingering and other response styles could be improved. In a desire to improve classification, clinicians often ignore the fact that malingering does not occur at predictable rates. The best available data (Rogers et al., 1996) found highly variable rates ( SD 14.44). Even within the same setting, rates are likely to vary markedly based on referral issues (see Rogers & Salekin, 1998). • Malingering is most likely to occur in persons with antiso- cial personality disorder (APD) . Psychopaths and persons with APD likely engage in deception (Rogers & Cruise, 2000), but no data indicate an increased likelihood for malingering in forensic settings. This unsupported as- sumption likely is based on a methodological artifact: Because most forensic studies are conducted in criminal settings, the facile connection between malingering and APD is understandable. • Malingering and mental disorders are mutually exclusive . Neither malingering nor mental disorders offer any natural immunity to the other. Some individuals with valid psy- chopathology “gild the lily” by adding feigned symp- toms. Most clinicians are willing to acknowledge the co-occurrence of malingering and mental disorders; how- ever, many forensic reports do not address the mental dis- orders after malingering has been determined. Applications to Forensic Practice Determinations of malingering often supersede all other clini- cal issues. When a forensic psychologist concludes that a person is malingering, this opinion is likely to invalidate all claims by that person, destroying his or her credibility. Because of its overshadowing importance, forensic psycholo- gists carry a further responsibility to ensure the accuracy of their conclusions with respect to malingering. We recom- mend that the classification of malingering should never rely on a single indicator. In addition to confirmation by multiple sources, forensic psychologists should systematically exclude alternative explanations (e.g., factitious disorders or irrelevant responding) in their determinations of malingering. To avoid misclassifications based solely on idiosyncratic data, Rogers and Shuman (2000) put forth the following forensic guideline: No determination of malingering should rest solely on tradi- tional interviews . The classification of malingering often appears dispositive of the verdict. Given this observation, what are the responsi- bilities of a forensic psychologist who believes that another expert’s conclusions about the presence of malingering were inaccurate? That psychologist bears the onerous responsibil- ity of comprehensively evaluating the issue of malingering. If the data continue to support his or her conclusion (i.e., the absence of malingering), then great care must be taken to marshal this evidence in a manner to convince the trier of fact. In general, forensic psychologists should assume an un- even playing field, with a much heavier burden of disproving than proving malingering. In sentencing and postverdict criminal evaluations, defen- siveness is often the preeminent issue. Courts and other adju- dicative bodies are concerned that dangerous persons not be released prematurely based on minimization of their psycho- logical impairment. Forensic psychologists must exercise a rigorous standard in conducting these evaluations, compa- rable to malingering determinations. EMPIRICAL ISSUES The clinical assessment of response styles rests solidly on their validation. As demonstrated in this section, no single re- search design is sufficient to validate measures of response style . With respect to preparing for testimony, Rogers (1997) provided a thorough review of these research designs. The purpose of this section is to provide forensic psychologists with a brief summary of research designs and their relevance to the assessment of response styles. Basic Designs Three designs predominate the validation of clinical mea- sures for the evaluation of malingering and defensiveness. Empirical Issues 113 Simulation Design Simulation studies use an analog design in which participants are randomly assigned to simulator and control conditions. For feigning studies, the addition of a clinical comparison sample is essential; otherwise, researchers cannot ascertain whether differences are attributable to feigning or to gen- uine disorders. With appropriate debriefing, the simulation design excels at internal validity but has limited external validity. Known-Groups Comparison Known-groups studies are conducted with independently classified malingerers who are compared with genuine pa- tients. The challenge is the identification of actual malinger- ers in sufficient numbers for research. The known-groups comparison excels at external validity but has limited internal validity. Differential Prevalence Comparison Differential prevalence studies assume that certain groups will have a higher prevalence of a specific response style (e.g., forensic patients for feigning and job applicants for defensiveness). Group differences have little practical sig- nificance without knowing what is the proportion of dis- simulation in different groups, or whether deviant scores represent dissimulation. Differential prevalence comparison fails to establish internal validity and has limited external validity. Bootstrapping Comparisons A fourth design, bootstrapping comparisons, recently has been observed in studies of feigned cognitive impairment. Persons identified by deviant scores on other measures of feigning are compared to those without these deviant scores. The key issue with bootstrapping comparisons is the selec- tion of measures with nearly perfect specificity, so that the “feigning” group does not contain genuine patients. Experi- mental rigor can be increased through the classification based on several measures representing different detection strategies. The best validation for measures of response styles is a combination of studies representing simulation design and known-groups comparisons. This combination maximizes both internal (simulation design) and external (known- groups comparison) validity. Forensic psychologists should take particular care to select measures with known-groups comparisons, because these studies are frequently omitted from the test validation. Incremental Validity Psychologists often believe that a convergence of findings across different measures contributes to incremental validity. As a counterposition, Sechrest (1963) demonstrated in his seminal article that the single best measure often is not im- proved by adding additional measures. As a forensic exam- ple, Kurtz and Meyer (1994) found that the SIRS was more accurate for the classification of feigning than either the MMPI-2 alone or the combination of the SIRS/MMPI-2. Forensic psychologists must decide whether to use the single best measure or a convergence of measures in establishing classificatory accuracy for response styles. We recommend that forensic psychologists employ multi- ple indices from different measures when malingering is sus- pected. Because the determination of malingering carries such grave consequences, its assessment should be compre- hensive. The results should be analyzed on two parameters: domain and detection strategies. Feigning can be divided into at least three broad domains (i.e., mental disorders, cognitive impairment, and medical illness) that differ substantially in clinical presentation. For each domain, detection strategies can be identified for the clinical classification of malingering; these detection strategies vary in the extent of their validation and accuracy of classification. To facilitate this analysis, subsequent sections of this chapter address domains and their respective detection strategies. Clinicians must be ready to grapple with both convergent and divergent results. What about convergent results? With consistent results from well-validated strategies derived from dissimilar mea- sures, forensic psychologists likely will have confidence in their conclusions about response styles. Such confidence should not be confused with increased accuracy (i.e., incre- mental validity); unless empirically demonstrated, psycholo- gists cannot conclude a higher level of accuracy. What about generally consistent results? The most com- mon finding in forensic evaluations is that most of the indica- tors agree; however, one or more indices of response styles do not fit with the other indicators. One temptation is to ignore or explain away the discrepant findings. A more pru- dent course is to evaluate the results, taking into account the accuracy of the measures and the validity of the detection strategies. For example, a “nonfeigning” classification on the SIRS has an excellent positive predictive power that is likely to outweigh a more nebulous elevation on an MMPI-2 validity scale. In addition, some detection strategies (e.g., symptom validity testing) are much more robust than others 114 Evaluation of Malingering and Deception (e.g., forced choice testing); their comparative validity can be taken into account in making determinations. What about inconsistent findings? The first possibility is that the results are domain-specific. For example, an evalua- tee with major depression (a mental disorder domain) may feign problems with attention, concentration, and immediate memory (a cognitive impairment domain) in the context of a disability evaluation. Sometimes, these cases can be resolved based on the accuracy of measures and relative validity of detection strategies. In other cases, the only logical decision is that the results are inconclusive. Forensic psychologists should be aware that some clinicians adopt a “fall-through-the-ice” mentality: Any failure (e.g., an indicator of feigning) is viewed as decisive evidence of a per- vasive response style. Like falling through the ice, the results are immediately catastrophic and summarily generalized. This mentality is empirically unwarranted and is probably more illuminating about the clinician than the evaluatee. MALINGERING OF MENTAL DISORDERS Detection Strategies Rogers (1997) and Rogers and Vitacco (in press) provide ex- tensive descriptions of detection strategies for feigned mental disorders. The purpose of this section is to highlight these pri- mary strategies. These strategies are important for under- standing how scales and specific indicators are utilized in the assessment of malingering. Using detection strategies, a con- ceptually based approach combines theory and empiricism. It offers judges and juries more than simply numbers and cut scores; it supplies the underlying logic and rationale for how the scales were constructed and the classification was reached. A distillation of eight detection strategies for feigned psy- chopathology is enumerated: 1. Rare Symptoms . Items in this strategy are very infre- quently endorsed by clinical populations. Malingerers often are unaware that certain symptoms are infrequently experienced. Rare symptoms represent one of the most robust detection strategies. 2. Improbable Symptoms . A minority of malingerers report or endorse symptoms that have a fantastic or preposterous quality. When a pattern of improbable symptoms is en- dorsed, the credibility of the evaluatee’s reporting is brought into question. 3. Symptom Combinations . Many symptoms commonly occur alone but rarely are paired together (e.g., grandios- ity and increased sleep). To foil this strategy, malingerers would need to have a sophisticated understanding of psychopathology. 4. Symptom Severity . Even severely impaired patients expe- rience only a discrete number of symptoms as “unbear- able.” Malingerers often are unable to estimate which symptoms and how many symptoms should have extreme severity. 5. Indiscriminant Symptom Endorsement . When asked about a broad array of psychological symptoms, some malinger- ers do not respond selectively but endorse a large propor- tion of symptoms. 6. Obvious versus Subtle Symptoms . Malingerers tend to en- dorse a high proportion of obvious symptoms (i.e., clearly indicative of a mental disorder). Obvious symptoms are either considered alone or in relation to subtle symptoms (i.e., “everyday” problems, not necessarily indicative of a mental disorder). When compared to genuine patients, malingerers often report a higher proportion of obvious symptoms. 7. Erroneous Stereotypes . Many persons have misconcep- tions about symptoms associated with mental disorders. When displaying erroneous stereotypes, persons feigning mental disorders can sometimes be detected. 8. Reported versus Observed Symptoms . Marked discrepan- cies between the person’s own account and clinical obser- vations appear useful in the detection of malingerers when standardized measures are used. The risk of this approach is that many genuine patients lack insight about their psy- chopathology. These eight detection strategies account for most of the systematic approaches to feigned mental disorders and constitute the framework for the evaluation of malingered symptomatology. Several additional strategies have been ex- plored. Morel (1998) used forced-choice testing (see section on Malingering and Cognitive Impairment) to test for feigned posttraumatic stress disorder; the bogus effects of emotional numbing were evaluated in a two-choice paradigm. Wildman and Wildman (1999) explored whether malingerers might be detected by their overly virtuous self-descriptions. Featured Measures A single chapter cannot comprehensively review the broad array of psychological measures adapted or developed for the assessment of feigned mental disorders. Therefore, this section addresses three featured measures that have been ex- tensively validated. Featured measures include two multi- scale inventories and one structured interview. Malingering of Mental Disorders 115 As a general caution, forensic psychologists should closely inspect test manuals and validation studies prior to using any test for feigned mental disorders. For example, we have observed numerous forensic reports attempting to use the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory III (MCMI-III; Millon, 1994; Millon, Davis, & Millon, 1997) to assess feign- ing. Is this use warranted based on a careful examination of the MCMI-III’s validation? The answer is clearly negative. For example, the debasement index is promoted as a fake-bad scale for detecting persons attempting to appear psychologi- cally impaired. Close inspection reveals the following: (a) both the 1994 and 1997 MCMI-III test manuals neglected the validation of the MCMI-III debasement index; (b) the MCMI-III debasement index appears confounded by psy- chopathology (i.e., 9 clinical scales correlate .75 in the normative sample); and © extrapolations from MCMI-II research would be inappropriate because only 19 of 46 (41.3%) MCMI-II items were retained on the MCMI-III debasement index. More than five years after the MCMI-III’s publication, research (Daubert & Metzler, 2000; Thomas- Peter, Jones, Campbell, & Oliver, 2000) is now beginning to emerge on the debasement index and feigning; more exten- sive research is needed before its use in forensic evaluations. Importantly, validational problems are not limited to the MCMI-III; forensic psychologists are urged to scrutinize closely the validation of all response style measures. Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 A large array of validity indices has been developed to evalu- ate whether MMPI-2 protocols have been feigned. Table 7.1 provides a summary of indices for the detection of both feigning and defensiveness. Summary data include the range of cut scores, available data on effect sizes, and a brief de- scription of scale development. Forensic psychologists are likely to be in a quandary about which MMPI-2 indices should be employed for the evalua- tion of malingering. Standard MMPI-2 texts provide con- flicting conclusions. Championing the traditionalist model, Butcher and Williams (1992) advocated the use of the F and Fb scales, virtually ignoring specialized scales for feigning. Graham (2000) also emphasized the use of traditional MMPI-2 indicators. However, he endorsed one specialized indicator (Fp) and discommended the use of other specialized indices. In stark contrast, Greene (1997, 2000) embraces a comprehensive model, with the use of both traditional and specialized indices of malingering. Both models are critically evaluated in subsequent sections. The traditionalist model of malingering, beyond history and convention, has several advantages that must be considered. In an MMPI-2 meta-analysis, Rogers, Sewell, and Salekin (1994) found the F and Fb had several of the largest effect sizes (2.56 and 1.85, respectively) for feigning when compared to clinical TABLE 7.1 Description of MMPI-2 Validity Indices for Feigning and Defensiveness Effect Sizes a Scale Items Cut Scores r a Feigning Defensive Scale Development Feigning Indices b F 60 8–30 1.00 2.56 Infrequency in normative samples. Fb 40 9–25 .86 1.85 Infrequency in normative samples. Fp 27 NA .75 NA Infrequency in inpatient samples. Dsr2 32 13–28 .61 c 1.54 Stereotypes of mental disorders. FBS 43 NA NA NA Rational: personal injury claims. LW 107 40–67 .84 1.38 Rational: urgent clinical issues. O-S NA d 74–190 .81 2.30 Rational: obvious versus subtle symptoms. Defensiveness Indices e L 15 6–9 .43 .94 Rational: borrowed from earlier scales. K 30 17–22 1.00 .90 Empirical: 30% for defensive patients. Mp 34 16–20 .48 f 1.42 Empirical: identify best impression. Wsd 33 21–23 .28 1.60 Empirical: socially desirable items. Esd 39 35–36 .76 .67 Rational: socially desirable items. S 52 NA .88 NA Differential prevalence with pilots. g Note : NA not available. a Correlations are reported in Greene (2000) for clinical samples between (a) feigning indices and Scale F and (b) defensiveness indices and Scale K. b Effect sizes and range of cut scores reported in MMPI-2 meta-analyses of feigning (Rogers, Sewell, & Salekin, 1994). c Dsr2 is not reported in Greene (2000); this estimate is based on the original 58-item Ds2 from which the Dsr2 was extracted. d Uses T-score transformations of subscales. e Effect sizes and range of cut scores reported by Baer, Wetter, and Berry (1992). Please note that this meta-analysis is based on the original MMPI and should be viewed only as a general benchmark for MMPI-2 performance. f Based on slightly modified Od scale. g Pilot applicants were assumed to have a high proportion of defensive persons; they were compared to a normative sample. 116 Evaluation of Malingering and Deception populations. Other research (Bagby, Buis, & Nicholson, 1995; Timbrook, Graham, Keiller, & Watts, 1993) has used hierar- chical multiple regression to evaluate whether the use of addi- tional validity indices would add incremental validity (i.e., account for more of the variance). These studies concluded that the F scale alone appeared to be the most predictive of malin- gering. A final advantage of the traditionalist model is its sim- plicity; forensic psychologists do not have to explain to the courts potential